• Nem Talált Eredményt

The findings of this study suggest that although there are some differences between the two foreign languages, findings contribute to the multifaceted and domain-spe-cific self-concept research. The present study might be conducive to self-concept research and theory regarding further empirical support for the operation of twofold multidimensionality and the I/E model of self-concept within a domain-specific level.

Regardless of the substantial findings of this study, future research is essential to investigate further twofold multidimensionality and the I/E model within the domain-specific level. Although the contribution of the present study to twofold multidimen-sionality and the I/E model is noteworthy, several limitations should be addressed.

First, the present study is cross-sectional and targeted only 10th-grade Azeri students.

Longitudinal studies are undoubtedly necessary to gain an accurate and deep intuitive understanding of the operation of the twofold multidimensionality and the I/E model of the reading self-concept structure within the foreign language domain. As studies (e.g., Marsh & Ayotte, 2003) explored the differentiation of self-concept according to the students’ age, it might be beneficial to compare the twofold multidimensional structure and the I/E model of reading self-concept within the foreign language domain between younger and older students. Moreover, longitudinal studies allow the investigation of the interrelation across time, as they enable the examination of self-concept and achievement relations that have been explored to be reciprocal (Marsh & Craven, 2006).

Second, to gain insight into the development of the twofold multidimensional structure and the I/E frame of reference model of reading self-concept within the for-eign language domain, future studies can integrate the five paradoxical frame of refer-ence (social comparison theory and frame of referrefer-ence, dimensional comparison theory, cross-cultural self-concept paradox, and big-fish–little-pond effect) and con-textual effects in reading self-concept formation that occur at different levels, as research has found psychological comparison processes influence self-perceptions and their relations to distal outcomes (see Marsh et al., 2020). Irrespective of the find-ings of this study that suggested the generalizability of the twofold multidimensional structure across the reading self-concepts related to students’ English as a first foreign language and Russian as a second foreign language, a closer inspection found subtle differences between the structures of two foreign languages. This inspection might serve as a basis for further studies on similarities and differences in reading self-con-cepts of two foreign languages.

It might be interesting to examine the applicability of the given conceptions to other domains such as listening, writing, and speaking. For example, the twofold multidi-mensional structure and the I/E frame of reference model of self-concept related to cognitive and affective components could be examined. This assumption would also allow for clarification of further multidimensionality within domain-specific self-con-cepts and to what extent this is theoretically and practically relevant. Therefore, researchers could examine the relationships of more domain-specific self-concepts with achievements of corresponding domains within different foreign languages.

Regarding further research on reading self-concept within the foreign language domain, it might also be beneficial to examine twofold multidimensionality of domain-specific and skill-related differentiation of motivational constructs such as self-effi-cacy. Although the construct of self-efficacy was found to be closely related to self-concept, which meant both constructs relate to students’ perceptions of ability, most studies (Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Jansen et al., 2015; Parker et al., 2014) found theoretical and empirical separation between self-concept and self-efficacy. While

self-concept applies to students’ self-perceptions of their ability related to a given domain in general and is past-oriented, self-efficacy refers to students’ expectations and confidence to perform a specific task successfully and is future-oriented. As Arens and Jansen (2016) indicated that self-concept and self-efficacy became more difficult to distinguish when both constructs were applied to a more specific content domain;

that task-orientation was considered as an essential part of the conceptualization of efficacy was partially confused with the specificity level of the item. The self-concept items used in the current study were self-conceptually distinct from the items of self-efficacy, such as item example used by Pajares et al. (1999) to assess writing:

“How sure are you that you can correctly spell all words in a one-page story or com-position?” (as cited in Arens & Jansen, 2016, p. 660). Hence, the empirical distinction of self-concept and self-efficacy on the level of specific domains would be compelling to explore in future studies.

The findings have significant practical implications for the design of interventions to improve students’ foreign language self-concept and achievement. Educators need to have a clear understanding of how cognitive and affective components of reading self-concepts in two foreign languages operate to improve students’ reading skills.

General perceptions of instructors about domain-specific academic competencies are noncompliant with learners’ perceptions, indicating that students’ competencies in dif-ferent domains are grounded on external assessment, and do not differ between and within domains (Marsh & Craven, 1997). Teachers are likely to assume that if students have a positive attitude toward reading, they will also have a positive belief about their ability and show better performance. Educators may also hold the belief that if stu-dents have a high concept in one foreign language, they will have a high self-concept in another foreign language (Yeung & Wong, 2004). Therefore, the findings of this study suggest that students hold distinct reading self-concepts for each foreign language. Students’ reading self-concept in English will not be affected by reading self-concept in Russian.

Similarly, if students perform well in English reading, these students’ reading self-concepts in Russian will not be influenced by English achievement. If students hold low beliefs related to their competence in a domain in which they show weak perfor-mance, teachers should intervene to support students’ learning by improving and strengthening their beliefs about their abilities in a given domain. The findings of this study are likewise worthwhile to curriculum developers, mainly in the foreign lan-guage field, to develop proper strategies to foster the growth of students’ beliefs about their reading ability. As the affective component is highly related to the cognitive component of reading self-concept, which is highly related to achievement, and if educators want to emphasize the importance of reading, they need to work on the affective component of reading self-concept of a foreign language in addition to the cognitive component.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by Stipendium Hungaricum Scholarship Program was launched by Hungarian Government; the program was managed by Tempus Public Foundation.

ORCID iD

Könül Karimova https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4840-3573

References

Abu-Hilal, M. M. (2005). Generality of self-perception models in Arab culture: Result from ten years of research. In H. Marsh, R. Craven, & D. McInerney (Eds.), The new frontiers of self research (pp. 157–196). Information Age Publishing.

Abu-Hilal, M. M., Abdelfattah, F. A., Alshumrani, S. A., Abduljabbar, A. S., & Marsh, H. W.

(2013). Construct validity of self-concept in TIMSS’s student background questionnaire:

A test of separation and conflation of cognitive and affective dimensions of self-concept among Saudi eighth graders. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28, 1201–

1220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0162-1

Arens, A. K., & Jansen, M. (2016). Self-concepts in reading, writing, listening and speak-ing: A multidimensional and hierarchical structure and its generalizability across native and foreign languages. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(5), 646–664. https://doi.

org/10.1037/edu0000081

Arens, A. K., Jansen, M., Preckel, F., Schmidt, I., & Brunner, M. (2020). The structure of aca-demic self-concept: A methodological review and empirical illustration of central models.

Review of Educational Research, 10(20), 1–39. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320972186 Arens, A. K., Yeung, A. S., Craven, R. G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2011). The twofold multidimen-sionality of academic self-concept: Domain specificity and separation between competence and affect components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 970–981. https://doi.

org/10.1037/a0025047

Arens, A. K., Yeung, A. S., Craven, R. G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2013). A short German ver-sion of the Self-Description Questionnaire I: Theoretical and empirical comparability.

International Journal of Research and Method in Education, 36(4), 415–438. https://doi.

org/10.1080/1743727X.2012.710503

Arens, A. K., Yeung, A. S., & Hasselhorn, M. (2014). Native language self-concept and read-ing self-concept: Same or different? Journal of Experimental Education, 82(2), 229–252.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2013.813362

Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2002). Three methods for studying developmental change: A case of reading skills and self-concept. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(3), 343–364. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709902320634447 Bong, M. (1998). Tests of the internal/external frames of reference model with subject-specific

academic self-efficacy and frame-specific academic self-concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(1), 102–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.1.102

Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How dif-ferent are they really? Educational Psychology Review, 15(1), 1–40. https://doi.

org/10.1023/A:1021302408382

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen

& J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). SAGE.

Calsyn, R. J., & Kenny, D. A. (1977). Self-concept of ability and perceived evaluation of oth-ers: Cause or effect of academic achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(2), 136–145. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.69.2.136

Cényelvi mérés. (2013/2014). feladatsorok és javitókulcsok [2013/2014 assessment of target languages: Test booklets and keys]. https://www.oktatas.hu/kozneveles/meresek/celny-elvi_meres/feladatsorok/celnymeres_2014

Chapman, J. W., & Tunmer, W. E. (1995). Development of young children’s reading self-concepts: An examination of emerging subcomponents and their relationship with read-ing achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(1), 154–167. https://doi.

org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.154

Chapman, J. W., & Tunmer, W. E. (1997). A longitudinal study of beginning reading achieve-ment and reading self-concept. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(3), 279–291.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1997.tb01244.x

Chapman, J. W., & Tunmer, W. E. (2003). Reading difficulties, reading-related self-percep-tions, and strategies for overcoming negative self-beliefs. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19(1), 5–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573560308205

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance.

Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834 Chen, F. F., Sousa, K. H., & West, S. G. (2005). Testing measurement invariance of second-order factor models. Structural Equation Modeling, 12(3), 471–492. https://doi.org/10.1207/

s15328007sem1203_7

Conradi, K., Jang, B. G., & McKenna, M. C. (2014). Motivation terminology in reading research: A conceptual review. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 127–164. https://doi.

org/10.1007/s10648-013-9245-z

Council of Europe. (2001). Common European framework of reference for languages: Learning, teaching, assessment. Cambridge University Press.

Csapó, B. (2014). A szegedi iskolai longitudinális program [The Hungarian educational longi-tudinal program]. In J. Pál & Z. Vajda (Eds.), Szegedi Egyetemi Tudástár 7Bölcsészet- és társadalomtudományok (pp. 117–166). Szegedi Egyetemi Kiadó.

Csapó, B., & Molnár, G. (2019). Online diagnostic assessment in support of personalized teach-ing and learnteach-ing: The eDia System. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1522. https://doi.

org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01522

Csapó, B., & Nikolov, M. (2009). The cognitive contribution to the development of proficiency in a foreign language. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 209–218. https://doi.

org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.01.002

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268. https://doi.

org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents’

achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295213003

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. Guilford Press.

Han, F. (2019). Self-concept and achievement in math among Australian primary stu-dents: Gender and cultural issues. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 603. https://doi.

org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00603

Hansford, B. C., & Hattie, J. A. (1982). The relationship between self and achievement/

performance measures. Review of Educational Research, 52(1), 123–142. https://doi.

org/10.2307/1170275

Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equa-tion modeling: Concepts, issues, and applicaequa-tions (pp. 76–99). SAGE.

Irwing, P. (1996). Evaluative and affective dimensions of self-concept: A test of construct valid-ity using structural equations modeling. Psychological Reports, 79(3), 1127–1138. https://

doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1996.79.3f.1127

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in chil-dren’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grade one through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00421 Jansen, M. F., Scherer, R., & Schroeders, U. (2015). Students’ self-concept and self-efficacy in

the sciences: Differential relations to antecedents and educational outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.11.002 Kasperski, R., Shany, M., & Katzir, T. (2016). The role of RAN and reading rate in predicting

reading self-concept. Reading and Writing, 29, 117–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9582-z

Katzir, T., Lesaux, N. K., & Kim, Y.-S. (2009). The role of reading self-concept and home literacy practices in fourth grade reading comprehension. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(3), 261–276. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9112-8 Kline, R. B. (2005). Methodology in the social sciences. Principles and practice of structural

equation modeling (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.

Lafontine, D., Dupont, V., Jaegers, D., & Schillings, P. (2019). Self-concept in reading: Factor structure, cross-cultural invariance and relationships with reading achievement in an inter-national context (PIRLS 2011). Studies in Educational Evaluation, 60, 78–89. https://doi.

org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2018.11.005

Marsh, H. W. (1986). Verbal and math self-concepts: An internal/external frame of reference model. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 129–149. https://doi.org/10.3102

%2F00028312023001129

Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept: Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 417–430. https://doi.

org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.417

Marsh, H. W. (1990). Self-Description Questionnaire–II manual. University of Western Sydney.

Marsh, H. W. (1993). The multidimensional structure of academic self-concept: Invariance over gender and age. American Educational Research Journal, 30(4), 841–860. https://doi.

org/10.2307/1163206

Marsh, H. W., & Ayotte, V. (2003). Do multiple dimensions of self-concept become more dif-ferentiated with age? The differential distinctiveness hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 687–706. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.687

Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. G. (1997). Academic self-concept: Beyond the dustbowl. In G. D.

Phye (Ed.), Educational psychology series. Handbook of classroom assessment: Learning, achievement, and adjustment (pp. 131–198). Academic Press.

Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond seductive pleasure and unidimensional per-spectives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 133–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/

j.1745-6916.2006.00010.x

Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Debus, R. (1998). Structure, stability, and development of young children’s self-concepts: A multicohort-multioccasion study. Child Development, 69, 1030–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06159.x

Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., & Debus, R. (1999). Separation of competency and affect com-ponents of multiple dimensions of academic self-concept: A developmental perspective.

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(4), 567–601. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23093373?seq=1

Marsh, H. W., & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on the structure of self-concept. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept: Developmental, social, and clinical con-siderations (pp. 38–90). John Wiley.

Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K.-T. (1996). Assessing goodness of fit: Is parsimony always desirable?

Journal of Experimental Education, 64, 364–390. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1996 .10806604

Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K.-T. (2004). Explaining paradoxical relations between academic self-concepts and achievements: Cross-cultural generalizability of the internal/external frame of reference predictions across 26 countries. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 56–67. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.56

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Grayson, D. (2005). Goodness of fit in structural equation mod-els. In A. Maydeu-Olivares & J. J. McArdle (Eds.), Multivariate applications book series.

Contemporary psychometrics: A festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald (pp. 275–340).

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Marsh, H. W., & Köller, O. (2004). Unification of theoretical models of academic self-concept/

achievement relations: Reunification of east and west German school systems after the fall of the Berlin Wall. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(3), 264–282. https://doi.

org/10.1016/S0361-476X(03)00034-1

Marsh, H. W., Kuyper, H., Seaton Parker, P. D., Morin, A. J. S., Möller, J., & Abduljabbar, A. S.

(2014). Dimensional comparison theory: An extension of the internal/external frame of ref-erence effect on academic self-concept formation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 326–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.08.003

Marsh, H. W., Martin, A. J., & Hau, K.-T. (2006). A multimethod perspective on self-concept research in educational psychology: A construct validity approach. In M. Eid & E. Diener (Eds.), Handbook of multimethod measurement in psychology (pp. 441–456). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11383-030

Marsh, H. W., Parker, P. D., Guo, J., Pekrun, R., & Basarkod, G. (2020). Psychological comparison processes and self-concept in relation to five distinct frames of reference effects: Pan-human cross-cultural generalizability over 68 countries. European Journal of Personality, 34, 180–202. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2232

Marsh, H. W., & Shavelson, R. (1985). Self-concept: Its multifaceted, hierarchical structure.

Educational Psychologist, 20, 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2003_1 Marsh, H. W., Xu, M., & Martin, A. J. (2012). Self-concept: A synergy of theory, method,

and application. In K. R. G. Harris, T. Urdan, C. B. McCormick, G. M. Sinatra, & J.

Sweller (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 427–458). American Psychological Association.

Marsh, H. W., & Yeung, A. S. (1997). Causal effects of academic self-concept on academic achievement: Structural equation models of longitudinal data. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.41

Marsh, H. W., & Yeung, A. S. (1998). Longitudinal structural equation models of academic self-concept and achievement: Gender differences in the development of math and English constructs. American Educational Research Journal, 35(4), 705–738. https://doi.

org/10.2307/1163464

Millsap, R. E. (2011). Statistical approaches to measurement invariance. Routledge.

Ministry of Education of Azerbaijan Republic. (2010). The new curriculum of state standards of specific subject programs (Decision 103). Ministry of Education of Azerbaijan Republic.

http://www.e-qanun.az/framework/19682

Ministry of Education of Azerbaijan Republic. (2012). Notes of training planning (Decision 427). Ministry of Education of Azerbaijan Republic. https://edu.gov.az/upload/file/emre-elave/2016/427/427-emre-elave11.pdf

Möller, J., & Pohlmann, B. (2010). Achievement differences and self-concept differences:

Stronger associations for above or below average students? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3). https://doi.org/10.1348/000709909X485234

Möller, J., Pohlmann, B., Köller, O., & Marsh, H. W. (2009). A meta-analytic path analy-sis of the internal/external frame of reference model of academic achievement and aca-demic self-concept. Review of Educational Research, 79(3), 1129–1167. https://doi.

org/10.3102%2F0034654309337522

Möller, J., Retelsdorf, J., Köller, O., & Marsh, H. W. (2011). The reciprocal internal/external frame of reference model an integration of models of relations between academic achieve-ment and self-concept. American Educational Research Journal, 48, 1315–1346. https://

doi.org/10.3102%2F0002831211419649

Morgan, P. L., & Fuchs, D. (2007). Is there a bidirectional relationship between children’s reading skills and reading motivation? Exceptional Children, 73(2), 165–183. https://doi.

org/10.1177/001440290707300203

Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (1998–2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.).

Nikolov, M., & Szabó, G. (2015). A study on Hungarian 6th and 8th graders’ proficiency in English and German at dual-language schools. In D. Halló & K. Károly (Eds.), Inspirations in foreign language teaching: Studies in applied linguistics. Language pedagogy and lan-guage teaching (pp. 184–206). Pearson Education.

Pajares, F., Miller, M. D., & Johnson, M. J. (1999). Gender differences in writing self-beliefs of elementary school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 50–61. https://doi.

org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.50

Parker, P. D., Marsh, H. W., Ciarrochi, J., Marshall, S., & Abduljabbar, A. S. (2014). Juxtaposing math self-efficacy and self-concept as predictors of long-term achievement outcomes.

Educational Psychology, 34(1), 29–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2013.797339 Pinxten, M., Marsh, H. W., De Fraine, B., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Van Damme, J. (2014).

Enjoying mathematics or feeling competent in mathematics? Reciprocal effects on math-ematics achievement and perceived math effort expenditure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 152–174. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12028

Raykov, T. (2009). Evaluation of scale reliability for unidimensional measures using latent variable modeling. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 42(3), 223–232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748175609344096

Retelsdorf, J., Köller, O., & Möller, J. (2011). On the effects of motivation on reading perfor-mance growth in secondary school. Learning and Instruction, 21(4), 550–559. https://doi.

org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.11.001

Retelsdorf, J., Köller, O., & Möller, J. (2014). Reading achievement and reading self-concept—

Testing the reciprocal effects model. Learning and Instruction, 29, 21–30. https://doi.

org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.004

Rutkowski, L., Gonzalez, E., Joncas, M., & von Davier, M. (2010). International large-scale assessment data: Issues in secondary analysis and reporting. Educational Researcher, 39(2), 142–151. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X10363170

Sass, D. A., & Schmitt, T. A. (2013). Testing measurement and structural invariance. In T. Teo (Ed.), Handbook of quantitative methods for educational research (pp. 315–345). Sense Publishers.

Schiefele, U., Schaffner, E., Möller, J., & Wigfield, A. (2012). Dimensions of reading motiva-tion and their relamotiva-tion to reading behavior and competence. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 427–462. https://doi.org/10.1002/RRQ.030

Shafiyeva, U., & Kennedy, S. (2010). English as a foreign language in Azerbaijan: English teaching in the post-Soviet era. English Today, 26(1), 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1017/

S0266078409990629

Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46(3), 407–441. https://psycnet.apa.org/

doi/10.2307/1170010

Shavelson, R. J., & Marsh, H. W. (1986). On the structure of self-concept. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Anxiety and cognitions (pp. 305–330). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Valentine, J. C., DuBois, D. L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relation between self-beliefs and aca-demic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 111–133.

https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3902_3

Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2020). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus (2nd ed.). John Wiley.

Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2020). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus (2nd ed.). John Wiley.