• Nem Talált Eredményt

The sense of hearing

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Ossza meg "The sense of hearing"

Copied!
22
0
0

Teljes szövegt

(1)

The sense of hearing

Tom DUKE

Cavendish Laboratory, University of Cambridge

PHYSBIO 2007 St Etienne de Tinée

Hearing

In terms of performance, hearing is the most remarkable of our senses

Frequency analysis: responds selectively to frequencies in range 20!20,000 Hz

Sensitivity: faintest audible sounds impart no more energy than thermal noise: 4 zJ per cycle

Dynamic range: responds and adapts over 12 orders of magnitude of energy: 0!120 dB

Dynamic range

20µPa 20Pa

0dB 30dB 60dB 90dB 120dB

displacement of air molecule at threshold of hearing ~ 0.1nm pneumatic drill conversation motorcycle

whisper rifle

The ear

external middle inner

auditory nerve

(2)

Middle ear

Impedance matches the air-filled ear canal with the fluid-filled cochlea Provides an acoustic gain owing to different areas of eardrum and stapes footplate

.

Cochlea

basilar membrane

Cochlea Organ of Corti

(3)

Theories of hearing

Helmholtz 1857

Detection apparatus consists of a set of strings of differing length which vibrate in resonance with the incoming sound

Theories of hearing

von Békésy 1923-47

Careful experiments on cadavers showed that a wave travels along the basilar membrane, and reaches a peak amplitude at a position that depends on frequency

Notion of place code

Theories of hearing

Gold 1948

Argued that damping by the fluid in the cochlea would not permit resonant oscillation and proposed that the ear is powered

Gold’s regeneration hypothesis

(4)

Regeneration hypothesis: An energy supply provides a feedback proportional to velocity, and in phase with it

near critical point, sharply tuned response at frequency

Gold’s regeneration hypothesis

‘!The magnitude of the feedback we require is so large as to come precariously close to cancelling the resistive losses. Some sort of self-regulating device would have to exist!’

Critical point

Otoacoustic emissions

Kemp ‘79; Manley & Köppl ‘98

The ear in not just a sound receiver; it also spontaneously emits sounds at a very low level

Hair cells in the bullfrog sacculus

source: Hudspeth

Detection apparatus

Hair bundle is composed of ~50 ‘stereocilia’, which lean against each other. Each sterocilium is a bundle of actin filaments, surrounded by the cell membrane, which tapers at the base. Adjacent stereocilia are connected by a fine

filament — the ‘tip link’

tip links

K+

kinocilium stereocilia

transduction channel

tip link (absent in mammals)

(5)

Detection apparatus

Hair bundle is composed of ~50 ‘stereocilia’, which lean against each other. Each sterocilium is a bundle of actin filaments, surrounded by the cell membrane, which tapers at the base. Adjacent stereocilia are connected by a fine

filament — the ‘tip link’

tip link

Mechano-chemo-electrical transduction

When bundle is pushed in direction of tallest stereocilium,

increased tension in tip links pulls open transduction channels

& admits K+

which depolarizes the membrane

& opens voltage-gated channels at the base of the cell

to nerve synapse

Rapidity of transduction process preserves information about timing of the signal

Transduction current

Active detetion by hair bundles

(6)

Active bundle movement

Crawford & Fettiplace ‘86; Howard & Hudspeth ‘87; Benser, Marquis & Hudspeth ‘96

twitch

spontaneous oscillation ?

Spontaneous hair-bundle oscillations

In the correct physiological conditions, hair bundles actively oscillate

Camalet, Duke, Jülicher & Prost ‘00 Vibration sensor is a nonlinear mechanical systemwhich can generate self-sustained oscillationsat a characteristic frequency

A feedback control mechanism maintains it on the verge of oscillating

Self-tuned critical oscillators

remarkable response properties at critical point Hopf bifurcation

control parameter

amplitude

Building a critical oscillator: inertial system

Gold ‘48

internal active force

critical point:

characteristic frequency:

(7)

Building a critical oscillator: non-inertial system

An internal active process can cause a heavily damped mechanical system to oscillate

Suppose effective elasticity can be made negative by changing control parameter C

Critical point at

active force with its own dynamics, coupled to displacement

Characteristic frequency

force:

displacement:

control parameter: C

bifurcation point:

Hopf resonance

gain diverges for weak stimuli

stimulus at characteristic frequency:

force:

displacement:

control parameter: C

bifurcation point:

Hopf resonance

active bandwidth

stimulus at different frequency:

if

Critical Hopf resonance : single tone response

Gain and active bandwidth depend on level of stimulus

frequency gain

f

c

f

a

! 0 db

20 db

(8)

Canonical equation

Eguiluz et al. ‘00

Response at ! = !c for different f

r

! /!c

Critical Hopf resonance : response in presence of noise

Camalet, Duke, Jülicher & Prost ‘00 phase-locking

Self-adjustment to critical point

Slow dynamics of control parameter, coupled to displacement, provides negative feedback which automatically adjusts system to the critical point

Eg. Ca2+ concentration is control parameter

C

C x

"

Cc

Self-tuned critical oscillations

stimulus

(9)

Spontaneous fluctuations

Martin, Hudspeth & Julicher ‘01

Auto-correlation function

Hair bundle response

Martin, Julicher & Hudspeth ‘01

Response of a frog hair bundle forced by a microneedle

Linear response

Martin, Hudspeth & Julicher ‘01 Response function # defined by:

Test of fluctuation-dissipation relation

Martin, Hudspeth & Julicher ‘01

At thermal equilibrium, the linear response is related to the fluctuations

(10)

Hair bundle response

Martin & Hudspeth ‘01

-2/3

Two adaptation mechanisms

fast process

Ca2+ binding to transduction channel

~ 1 ms

slow process

movement of myosin-1C motors

~ 100 ms

Negative elasticity

Instantaneous force response to an applied displacement

Martin & Hudspeth ’00

Channel gating compliance

Suppose channel incorporates a lever arm

opening of channel can substantially reduce the tension in the tip link

• negative elasticity if

(11)

Channel gating compliance Influence of motors

Ca2+ binds to motors;

causes them to produce less force Ca2+ released from motors;

causes them to produce more force

channels closed channels open

Physical basis of oscillations

Tinévez & Martin ‘06

force/displacement

bundle position

motor position

transduction current

Self-tuning mechanism ?

Tinévez & Martin ‘06 Maybe Ca2+ affects rigidity of tip-links, or size of channel movement

(12)

Vilfan & Duke ‘02

• Oscillations generated by interaction of Ca2+ with transduction channels

Alternative physical basis of self-tuned oscillations

Ca Ca

Vilfan & Duke ‘02

• Oscillations generated by interaction of Ca2+ with transduction channels

frequency

depends on bundle geometry

Alternative physical basis of self-tuned oscillations

Ca2+

motors

Self-tuning accomplished by movement

of molecular motors, regulated by Ca2+

An active role for the kinocilium ?

• kinocilium is motile (Rüsch & Thurm ‘90)

• axoneme can vibrate at ~1000 Hz (Kamimura & Kamiya ‘89)

• but spontaneous oscillations of frog hairbundles still occur when the bundle is knocked down (Martin) K+

kinocilium stereocilia

transduction channel tip link

detection

apparatus force generator

Hearing in fruit flies

The antenna of mosquitoes sense both odorants and sounds

(13)

Spontaneous oscillations and nonlinear response of antennae

Göpfert ‘03

Spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in reptiles

Köppl & Manley ‘93

level (dB)

frequency (Hz)

Reptilian inner ear

Köppl & Manley ‘93

sallet: massive structure bobtail lizard

Coupled critical oscillators

.

!

i-1

!

i

!

i+1

m

i-1

m

i

m

i+1

real for dissipative coupling

Vilfan & Duke

(14)

Spontaneous oscillations of a coupled chain

Ermentrout & Koppel ‘84

!

i

oscillation frequency

Cochlear mechanics

.

Human cochlea

basilar membrane cochlea

outer hair cells inner hair cells

tectoral membrane

Incoming sound excites a slow travelling wave on the basilar membrane

• location of peak depends on frequency

Place code

Békésy’s travelling wave

(15)

Cochlear travelling wave

• sound sets fluid into motion

• variation in flow rate is accommodated by movement of membrane

• membrane acceleration is caused by difference in fluid pressure oval window

round window

helicotrema

Zwislocki ‘48

membrane displacement h

pressure difference p = P1 - P2 difference in flows j = J1 - J2

• fluid flow

• incompressibility

• membrane response

wave velocity

Travelling wave: one-dimensional model

Travelling wave: damped dispersive waves

Stiffness decreases by two orders of magnitude from base to apex

as wave propagates, it slows down & its amplitude increases Wave peaks when damping becomes significant

Location of peak depends on frequency

4.6 kHz

1.3 kHz

370 Hz

Sound level required to see an observable response at a given place

Tuning curve

frequency (kHz)

level (dB)

(16)

Tuning curve

Lighthill ‘81 In order to obtain a very localized peak, require:

• only waves with frequency lower than

!

c

(x)

carry energy past point

x

• as wave approaches characteristic place, velocity of energy transport falls to zero

• velocity drops so fast that there is time to dissipate all energy before wave reaches characteristic place

energy piles up at characteristic place

Energy flow in the cochlea

Kachar et al. ‘86; Ashmore ‘87

Outer hair cells

outer hair cells inner hair cells

Critical oscillators are ranged along basilar membrane, and are positioned so that they can drive its motion

• characteristic frequency diminishes from base to apex, spanning the audible range

Partition is an excitable medium with a nonlinear active response

• active oscillators negate friction and damping at the characteristic place where the oscillator frequency matches the sound frequency

Active cochlear partition

(17)

Active travelling wave: waveforms

4.6 kHz

1.3 kHz

370 Hz

Basilar membrane motion

Rhode ‘71; Ruggero et al. ‘97; Russell & Nilsen ‘97

BM response is nonlinear

1/3 1

Basilar membrane motion

Rhode ‘71; Ruggero et al. ‘97; Russell & Nilsen ‘97

BM response is nonlinear

Lighthill ‘81 Cochlea is an unusual type of waveguide:

• only waves with frequency lower than

!

c

(x)

carry energy past point

x

• as wave approaches characteristic place, velocity of energy transport falls to zero

• velocity drops so fast that there is time to dissipate all energy before wave reaches characteristic place

energy piles up at characteristic place

Energy flow in the cochlea

(18)

Nonlinearities due to active amplification

A critical oscillator is ideal for detecting a single tone …

… but it causes tones of different frequency to interfere

Response to multiple tones:

Psychoacoustic effects

& their physical origin

Two-tone suppression

Two-tone suppression f1

presence of a second tone diminishes the neural response to the first tone

f2

f1 & f2

suppression is greater if frequencies are close

Psychoacoustics & auditory illusions

Range of phenomena associated with multiple tones: f1, f2, ...

Combination tones: frequencies not present in the stimulus can be heard, Tartini 1714 most prominently 2f1 - f2

Residue pitch: frequencies close to, but not necessarily equal to Schouten 1938 "f =f2 - f1 can be heard

Consonance: two musical notes whose frequencies are in a

Pythagoras simple integer ratio sound pleasant

basis of diatonic scale

(19)

Two-tone suppression

Presence of second tone can extinguish the nonlinear amplification

= 0 ! 0

Distortion products

Nonlinearities create a characteristic spectrum of distortion products

Active travelling wave:

two-tone response

distance along cochlea

2!1-!2 !1

!2

frequency component

Active travelling wave: two-tone response

Andor

location vibrating almost exclusively at 2!1-!2

distortion product can be heard

!

1

!

2

2 !

1

- !

2

(20)

Neural representation of pitch and level

Two competing theories:

Place code pitch represented by the place in the cochlea where nerves fire most frequently

level represented by firing rate at that place

Time code pitch represented by time interval between spikes

level represented by number of nerves firing at an elevated rate

Auditory nerve: spontaneous activity

Liberman ‘77

spontaneous rate (spikes/sec) spontaneous rate (spikes/sec)

threshold (dB)

number of units

Inference of pitch and level

Jülicher, Andor & Duke ‘01

Model • a spike is elicited whenever the hair bundle deflection traverses a thresholdthere are hair cells with a wide range of

thresholds for each characteristic frequency

Algorithm • construct histogram of inter-spike intervals T, summing over all hair cells in the cochlea

perceived tones correspond to peaks in histogram and are assigned pitch 1/T

perceived level corresponds to height of peak

First passage time to traversal of threshold

Temporal coding permits detection of stimuli 20 dB below rate-threshold

(21)

First passage time to traversal of threshold

Temporal coding permits detection of stimuli

20 dB below rate-threshold spontaneous

high level stimulus

Distortion products: spectra & waveforms

Spike timings: two tones

.

interval / s

firing prob. density

0.009s ~ 110 Hz 0.045s ~ 22 Hz

residue pitch high threshold neuron

Two-tone stochastic resonance

Chialvo et al. ‘02

(22)

Nature of dissonance

Helmholtz ascribed dissonance to close mis-matches in frequency between harmonics 5:4

4:3

"2:1

log(frequency)

Kaestner 1909

Integer ratios of frequencies are preferred when notes are played on musical instruments

For pure tones, there is no such preference, but only a dislike of close frequencies

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 f / f

! pure tones

+ harmonics

Dissonance: psychophysical experiments

3:2 5:3 4:3 5:4

6:5 8:5

1:1 2:1

pleasantness

loudness

time

0.8 1 1.2 1.4

inferred pitch T / f-1 1 1.30

1.03 f / f 2 1

1.10

Pitch discrimination

Two tones are heard as a single tone if they differ in frequency by less than 5%

Two tones are interpreted ambiguously if they differ in frequency by 5 - 15%

Two tones can be clearly distinguished if they differ in frequency by more than 15%

Dissonance: a physical interpretation

Close frequency mis-matches generate complicated hair bundle responses

The difficulty of inferring frequency components from partial information about a complex waveform results in an indeterminacy of pitch

1 1.1 1.2 1.3

1 1.1 1.2 1.3

f / f2 1 rough

‘What distinguishes dissonances from consonances is not a greater or lesser degree of beauty, but a greater or lesser degree of comprehensibility’

Arnold Schoenberg

inferred pitch

Hivatkozások

KAPCSOLÓDÓ DOKUMENTUMOK

The upper reaches of the river network have greater kinetic energy due to the large amount of water, and the preferential growth of branches at the tips is the most obvious4. With

It is also important to note that the Lévy scale α parameter is above 1 at the whole energy range so we are still far from the conjectured critical point value. Among all

Friedrichs had pointed out that wave propagation was intimately related to energy considerations, so again, Hilbert space was the optimal context for the study of waves... (3.6) We

At the pile load tests only total capacity of the piles is measured, so it was necessary to calculate the base resistance and the shaft resistance of the piles separately, based on

3 Spontaneity in this sense can be interpreted as the property of spontaneous writing on the Internet (cf. According to my knowledge, there is no analysis of the

This characteristic behaviour of the so called 'shape memory' alloys is based on the martensitic transformation, the ~( phase (austenite) stable at higher

For instance, with regard to sandy soil, when the velocity of wave propagation reaches to 250 meters per sec- ond, it is possible to observe three peaks with the approximate strain

The examined characteristics (crystal structure, glow curve, fading, dose characteristic, energy dependence) of the thermoluminescent phos- phor produced by each method did